|
|
ORIGINAL ARTICLE |
|
Year : 2022 | Volume
: 39
| Issue : 4 | Page : 183-190 |
|
Tinnitus and Underlying Theoretical Mechanism: The Key and Lock?
Serpil Mungan Durankaya1, Asli Cakir Cetin2, Basak Mutlu3, Selhan Gurkan1, Gunay Kirkim1, Mustafa Bulent Serbetcioglu4
1 Department of Audiometry, Vocational School of Health Services, Dokuz Eylül University; Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Hearing, Speech and Balance Unit, Dokuz Eylul University Hospital, Istanbul, Turkey 2 Department of Otorhinolaryngology, School of Medicine, Dokuz Eylul University, Izmir, Turkey 3 Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Hearing, Speech and Balance Unit, Dokuz Eylul University Hospital, Izmir; Department of Audiology, Faculty of Health Sciences, Istanbul Medeniyet University, Istanbul, Turkey 4 Department of Otorhinolaryngology, School of Medicine, Dokuz Eylul University, Izmir; Department of Audiology, Faculty of Health Sciences, Istanbul Medipol University, Istanbul, Turkey
Date of Submission | 24-Mar-2022 |
Date of Decision | 26-Jul-2022 |
Date of Acceptance | 01-Aug-2022 |
Date of Web Publication | 19-Dec-2022 |
Correspondence Address: Serpil Mungan Durankaya Department of Audiometry, Vocational School of Health Services, Dokuz Eylül University, 35320 Balçova-Izmir Turkey
 Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None
DOI: 10.4103/nsn.nsn_55_22
Background and Aim: To evaluate the association between psychoacoustical characteristics of tinnitus and audiogram configurations and reveal which theoretical mechanism dominates tinnitus. Materials and Methods: The medical charts of 110 adult participants' 164 ears with tinnitus were retrospectively reviewed. Audiological results, edge frequency, and psychoacoustical characteristics of tinnitus were assessed. Participants were divided into two groups as follows: normal hearing (NH) and sensorineural hearing loss (SNHL). Results: No significant relationship was observed between age, gender, tinnitus pitch, and loudness between the two groups. In the SNHL group, there was a weak positive correlation between tinnitus pitch and frequency of maximum hearing loss (FMHL), and a strong positive correlation between the mean tinnitus loudness at the tinnitus pitch and FMHL. Besides, the edge frequency was positively and weakly correlated with the tinnitus pitch and FMHL. No statistically significant difference was observed between the groups regarding the tinnitus pitch. However, tinnitus loudness was statistically higher in the NH group. No relationship was observed between the audiogram shapes and tinnitus timbre, pitch, and FMHL. In addition, the most likened tinnitus timbre was found to be tonal/whistle in both groups. A moderate positive correlation was observed between the tinnitus pitch and edge frequency in the gradual slope audiograms. Conclusions: The findings obtained in this study supported homeostatic plasticity theories for the SNHL group, and hidden hearing loss for the NH group.
Keywords: Edge frequency, tinnitus, tinnitus mechanisms, tinnitus pitch
How to cite this article: Durankaya SM, Cetin AC, Mutlu B, Gurkan S, Kirkim G, Serbetcioglu MB. Tinnitus and Underlying Theoretical Mechanism: The Key and Lock?. Neurol Sci Neurophysiol 2022;39:183-90 |
How to cite this URL: Durankaya SM, Cetin AC, Mutlu B, Gurkan S, Kirkim G, Serbetcioglu MB. Tinnitus and Underlying Theoretical Mechanism: The Key and Lock?. Neurol Sci Neurophysiol [serial online] 2022 [cited 2023 May 29];39:183-90. Available from: http://www.nsnjournal.org/text.asp?2022/39/4/183/364423 |
Introduction | |  |
Tinnitus is a common and distressing otologic symptoms of which pathogenesis has still not been entirely illuminated.[1],[2] The observations such that the majority of patients with tinnitus also suffer from various degrees of hearing loss and the increase in the severity of tinnitus as hearing deteriorates emerge the hypothesis that the hearing loss may be a potential trigger for tinnitus.[3],[4] Psychoacoustic assessment of tinnitus can be used to evaluate the mechanisms of tinnitus generation. Many studies have investigated the relationship between the psychoacoustical characteristics of tinnitus and audiometric characteristic of hearing loss for decades and theorized several mechanisms, but the results have some controversial issues.[5],[6],[7],[8],[9],[10],[11] However, it has not been clarified yet whether there is a relationship between the tinnitus pitch and the audiometric edge,[4],[5],[6],[7],[10],[12],[13],[14] which remained to be investigated.
Objective
This study aimed to evaluate the association between the psychoacoustic parameters of tinnitus and audiological data and reveal which mechanism is responsible for the emergence of tinnitus.
Materials and Methods | |  |
Ethical considerations and study design
This retrospective nonrandomized clinical research was approved by Dokuz Eylül University, Ethical Committee (protocol number: 2019/17-15). In this study, all interventions involving human volunteers were also in accordance with the ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Study size, setting, participants, and eligibility
The medical charts of the patients who experienced tinnitus for at least 3 months and were admitted to the department of Otorhinolaryngology, from January 1, 2016, to December 31, 2019, were reviewed (n = 256). The exclusion criteria were fluctuating hearing loss, conductive hearing loss, stapedial otosclerosis, retrocochlear pathologies, history of otological surgery, head trauma, ototoxicity, neurological disorders, presence of external, middle and inner ear infections, any abnormalities in otomicroscopic examination, other results than Type A tympanogram in 226 Hz tympanometry (Madsen Zodiac Type 1096, GN Otometrics, Denmark), and aged under 18 years. Patients with pulsatile and objective tinnitus were not eligible to participate in this study and therefore did not include. Finally, eligible 110 participants were enrolled in the study. The participants were divided into two groups as follows: normal hearing (NH) and sensorineural hearing loss (SNHL). None of the patients had unilateral SNHL.
Data sources and variables
The initial audiological evaluation was comprised air and bone conduction hearing thresholds at 250–16000 Hz, word recognition score, and speech reception thresholds (Interacoustics AC40, Otometrics, Denmark). Pitch and loudness matching were also performed using a multifrequency clinical audiometer (Interacoustics AC40, Otometrics, Denmark). The means of air conduction (AC) hearing thresholds at 500, 1000, and 2000 Hz; 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz; and 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz were defined as PTA1, PTA2, and PTA3, respectively. The NH was defined as having a pure-tone average at 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz thresholds better than 25 dB HL.
The hearing threshold test results were classified according to seven different audiometric configurations as follows: flat, high-frequency gradual slope (HFGS), high-frequency steep slope (HFSS), notched, low-frequency hearing loss (LFHL), inverted U-shape, and the others [Figure 1].[6] | Figure 1: The definition of the audiogram configurations. Steep slope (a-left ear), low frequency hearing loss (a-right ear), flat type (b-left ear), gradual slope (1b-right ear), notch type (c-left ear) and inverted U-shape (c-right ear)
Click here to view |
The difference between the maximum and minimum hearing threshold was equal or higher than 50 dB Hearing Level (HL) in the HFSS [[Figure 1]a-left ear], and <50 dB HL in the HFGS types [[Figure 1]b-right ear], respectively. The difference between low-frequency (the mean of 250, 500, and 1000 Hz) and high-frequency (the mean of 2000, 3000, 4000, 6000, and 8000 Hz) hearing thresholds was defined to be equal or more than 15 dB HL in the LFHL type [[Figure 1]a-right ear]. The difference between the maximum and minimum hearing thresholds was <25 dB HL in the flat type [[Figure 1]b-left ear]. The notch type had a notch at any frequency between 3000 and 6000 Hz. The notch had to be at least 25 dB worse than the threshold at 1000 Hz [[Figure 1]c-left ear]. The hearing threshold at mid-frequency (1000 and/or 2000 Hz) had to be at least 20 dB HL lower than the hearing thresholds at low (250 and 500) and high (4000–8000 Hz) frequencies in the Inverted U-shape [[Figure 1]c-right ear].
The edge frequencies of the notch, gradual slope, and steep slope audiograms were determined according to two criteria. According to the first criterion, if the difference between the thresholds at consecutive two octave or half octave frequencies is 15 dB or more, the first of these frequencies is considered as the edge frequency. According to the second criterion, if the threshold difference of the first and third frequencies from three consecutive frequencies is 25 dB or more, the first frequency is defined as the edge frequency [Figure 2].
The side of the tinnitus ear was decided by asking in which ear the participant felt tinnitus most frequently and loudest. In the case of bilateral symmetrical tinnitus perception, better hearing ear were assigned to be tinnitus-ear. Moreover, in the case of bilateral symmetrical hearing loss with similar tinnitus perception, the side decision was left to patient preference. Participants were assured to fully understand what pitch and loudness represent. The sound most similar to tinnitus, either pure tone or narrow-band noise, was initially presented one-octave frequency below the predicted tinnitus pitch. Interoctave frequencies were also utilized to provide exact pitch matching when needed. Tinnitus pitch matching was performed at 250–16000 Hz (Interacoustics AC40, Otometrics, Denmark). Tinnitus loudness matching was assessed by presenting an initial sound stimulus 10 dB above the AC hearing threshold at the individual's tinnitus pitch. When required, the threshold of the stimulus was decreased in 5 dB steps and increased in 1 dB steps.[13] To determine tinnitus timbre, patients were administered a tinnitus questionnaire form, in which they were asked to declare the most resembled tone with their tinnitus among six different sounds, including whistle, cicada, engine, wind, water boiling, and the others.
Statistical analysis
The statistical analysis was carried out using the SPSS 24.0 (IBM, Armonk, New York, USA) software. The numerical results were expressed as mean ± standard deviation, minimum, and maximum values, and the categorical variables were presented as percentages. Kolmogorov–Smirnov normality test was used to check the distribution pattern of each variable. The psychoacoustic characteristic of tinnitus, demographic features, and audiological data of the patients were measured using descriptive statistics. The impact of age, gender, and audiological data on the psychoacoustic properties of tinnitus were calculated by Spearman's rank correlations, Kruskal–Wallis, and Mann–Whitney U-tests, separately. The minimum significance level P = 0.05, and the results were reported in 95% confidence interval.
Bias
The individuals' audiological test battery was achieved by a blinded clinical audiologist Selhan Gurkan (SG). The tinnitus assessment was made by another nonblinded clinical audiologist Serpil Mungan Durankaya (SMD). Data recording and statistical analysis were accomplished by blinded authors (ACC and BM).
Results | |  |
Participants and descriptive data
The study sample consisted of 110 patients' (70 (63.6%) male, 40 (36.4%) female) 164 ears with tinnitus. The distribution of age and gender is shown in [Figure 3]. | Figure 3: The distribution of the age and gender of the participants. SNHL: Sensorineural hearing loss, NH: Normal hearing
Click here to view |
The demographical and audiological characteristics of the participants are demonstrated in [Table 1]. | Table 1: The demographical and audiological characteristics of the participants
Click here to view |
Outcome data and main results
Audiological results
The mean age of the SNHL group was significantly higher than the NH group (P = 0.000). Furthermore, the Spearman's rank correlation test demonstrated significant and positive moderate correlations between age and PTA1 (r = 0.40, P = 0.000), PTA2 (r = 0.38, P = 0.000) and PTA3 values (r = 0.41, P = 0.000), respectively. No significant relationship was observed between age, gender, and psychoacoustic characteristics of tinnitus (pitch and loudness) (P > 0.05).
The frequency-specific mean AC thresholds of the NH and SNHL groups are summarized in [Figure 4]. | Figure 4: Frequency-specific hearing threshold averages in subjects with sensorineural hearing loss (n = 119) and normal hearing (n = 45)
Click here to view |
Tinnitus characteristics and relevance to the audiological results and other analyses
The psychoacoustic characteristics of tinnitus and edge frequencies in seven different audiogram configurations in the SNHL and NH groups are indicated in [Table 2]. | Table 2: The psychoacoustic characteristics of tinnitus and edge frequencies in seven different audiogram configurations in the sensorineural hearing loss (n=119) and normal hearing (n=45) groups
Click here to view |
In the SNHL group, the edge frequency was close to 2000 Hz where hearing loss begins [Figure 4]. Tinnitus pitch was 1.34 ± 0.8 octaves above the edge frequency (P = 0.000). There were weak positive correlations between edge frequency and the tinnitus pitch (r = 0.27 P = 0.006), and the frequency of maximum hearing loss (FMHL) (r = 0.32 P = 0.001), respectively. The mean pitch of tinnitus (6078 ± 2946 Hz) was approximate to the mean FMHL (5992 ± 1972 Hz, P > 0.05). There was a weak positive correlation between the tinnitus pitch and FMHL (r = 0.31, P = 0.001). Furthermore, the mean tinnitus loudness at the tinnitus pitch (62.2 ± 16.6 dB HL) was close to the hearing threshold at the FMHL (56.6 ± 15.3 dB HL) with a strong positive correlation (r = 0.68, P = 0.000).
There was no statistically significant difference between the tinnitus pitch of the NH (5678 Hz) and SNHL (6078 Hz) groups (P = 0.747). However, the tinnitus loudness was statistically higher in NH (13.7 ± 8.0 dB SL) group compared to the SNHL group (9.7 ± 5.8 dB SL) (P = 0.001). A moderate negative correlation was observed between the hearing threshold at the FMHL and tinnitus loudness (dB SL) (r = −0.4 P = 0.000).
The most frequently likened tinnitus timbre was the tonal/whistle sound in both groups. The psychoacoustic features of tinnitus regarding tinnitus timbre are summarized in [Table 3]. | Table 3: The distribution of timbre, pitch, and loudness of tinnitus in the sensorineural hearing loss (n=119) and normal hearing (n=45) groups
Click here to view |
The tinnitus pitch was significantly higher in participants with tonal/whistle tinnitus timbre (P = 0.000) in both groups. No relationship was observed between the tinnitus timbre and the edge frequency (r = −0.9, P = 0.349) in both groups. No relationship was observed between the audiogram shapes and tinnitus timbre (r = 0.07, P = 0.400), tinnitus pitch (r = 0.05 P = 0.512) and the FMHL (r = −0.16 P = 0.083). However, a significant positive moderate correlation (r = 0.4, P = 0.003) was observed between the tinnitus pitch and the edge frequency in the gradual slope audiograms.
The distribution of the psychoacoustic parameters of tinnitus regarding laterality is shown in [Table 4]. Accordingly, there was no significant difference between the groups (P = 0.937). No significant difference was recognized regarding the tinnitus pitch, tinnitus loudness, edge frequency, tinnitus timbre, age, and gender between the right and left ears in both groups (P > 0.5 for each). | Table 4: The loudness, pitch, and edge frequency of tinnitus according to the side of tinnitus ear in the sensorineural hearing loss (n=119) and normal hearing (n=45) groups
Click here to view |
Discussion | |  |
Key results
In this research, we aimed to assess and illuminate the predominant pathophysiological process underlying tinnitus. Since the tinnitus pitch in our SNHL group was near to FMHL, we can suggest the predominance of the homeostatic plasticity theory. On the other hand, our NH group can be explained by the hidden hearing loss theory.
Interpretation
Tinnitus distributes in a wide spectrum regarding the audiogram configurations, pitch, loudness, and timbre.[3],[4],[5],[6],[7],[8],[9],[10],[12],[14] Considering audiological properties, the vast majority of our SNHL group consisted of high-frequency hearing loss (gradual slope and steep slope), compatible with the literature.[15],[16],[17],[18] Age and gender were not found to affect the psychoacoustic properties of tinnitus, as previously mentioned.[9],[19] Our SNHL group was elder than the NH group, similar to previous studies, which indicated presbytinnitus.[15],[16],[20],[21] Our results indicated that men tended to suffer from tinnitus more than women possibly because of higher vulnerability to occupational noise exposure, which has been detailed in the literature.[15],[16],[21],[22],[23],[24]
The loudness of tinnitus was found as 9.7 and 13.7 dB SL in SNHL and NH groups in our cohort, respectively. According to some previous reports, tinnitus loudness was lower than 10 dB SL,[3],[9],[18],[25] whereas in the others, it was found to be higher.[16],[19],[22],[26] Our results demonstrated a negative correlation between the hearing threshold and tinnitus loudness. There are published results both supporting[22] and not supporting[27] our findings. Presumably, as the patient hears better, the tinnitus perceptibility may increase, and thus, tinnitus may become louder.[22] From our perspective, this might be the case in the NH group.
In contrast, hearing loss has not been shown to affect tinnitus severity according to several studies.[15],[28] Searchfield et al., measured Tinnitus Severity Index for this purpose and did not find any significant correlation between the tinnitus severity and pure tone audiogram.[29] Similarly, a recent study[28] declared no correlation between tinnitus annoyance and Tinnitus Handicap Index results. However, the heterogeneity of the sample in these studies and the subjective nature of the questionnaires should also be considered while interpreting the results.[30]
Despite opponents,[15],[22] some authors[30] advocate an association between tinnitus loudness and hearing loss. Even, tinnitus loudness was found to be lower in individuals with uni- or bilateral hearing loss compared to NH matches.[13] This finding was explained by the presence of loudness recruitment which is a typical consequence of outer hair cell dysfunction and manifested by an exaggerated increase in perceived loudness despite a slight increase in stimulus intensity. The most reasonable mechanism underlying recruitment is the cochlear nonlinearity and it is not related to mood or level of anxiety.[13]
Concerning tinnitus timbre, the participants with tonal tinnitus had higher tinnitus pitch (6291 Hz for NH, 6526 Hz for SNHL), similar to previous data.[6],[16],[24] However, no relevance was observed between the configuration of the audiogram and tinnitus timbre, as previously mentioned.[6]
Recent literature has suggested that particularly in cases of tonal tinnitus, the tinnitus pitch should be close to an edge frequency, which corresponds to a boundary between a region of normal or near-NH and hearing loss.[4],[6],[7],[9] The edge frequency reflects the transition zone between the damaged and undamaged region of the auditory system. It is typically approximate to where hearing loss begins,[14] consistent with our findings. The edge frequency (2285 ± 1023 Hz) identified in our research was 1.34 ± 0.8 octaves lower than the tinnitus pitch [Table 1]. According to the edge effect theory, because of the disconnected inputs of the tonotopic axis, an imbalanced lateral inhibition occurs in the boundary of the damaged and undamaged zone, which may lead to a cortical reorganization and over-representation of the edge-frequencies, and finally tinnitus.[10] The reason why the tinnitus pitch is within the hearing loss zone but far away from the edge frequency in our cohort may be explained by the existence of homeostatic plasticity rather than cortical reorganization and edge effect.[5],[9],[10],[31],[32]
As reported in the literature,[4],[5],[6],[8],[10],[12],[14],[18],[22],[23] tinnitus pitch falls within high frequencies in audiogram and usually corresponds to the FMHL, which was also valid in our cohort [Table 1]. There are published results both supporting[10],[15] and not supporting[6],[22] our findings. The frequency proximity of maximum hearing loss to tinnitus pitch has been discussed by a theoretical model called homeostatic plasticity.[8],[9],[12],[18],[31],[32],[33] Homeostatic plasticity basically reflects the increased spontaneous fire rate and synchrony in the upper auditory pathway, rather than the temporal cortex which is caused by peripheral deafferentation. This process manifests in chronic tinnitus.[6] The tinnitus of our SNHL group can be explained by this theory. Even, it was proposed that this mechanism can be responsible for tinnitus in patients with NH, whose otoacoustic emissions (OAEs) are abnormal or absent.[34] However, the lack of OAEs in our cohort complicates to make an association between the tinnitus of the NH group and this theory. Homeostatic plasticity may also explain the somatosensorial alterations of tinnitus, since the dorsal cochlear nucleus and medial geniculate body have somatic and limbic inputs, respectively.[34],[35],[36] However, we did not assess such properties of tinnitus in our study. Nevertheless, we found weak correlations between both FMHL– tinnitus pitch and edge frequency– tinnitus pitch pairs. Hence, we can only interpret our data based on the numerical proximity of the tinnitus pitch and FMHL.
Another theory, hidden hearing loss, has been introduced by Schaette and McAlpine to identify the mechanism underlying tinnitus without hearing loss.[37] This theory assumes that tinnitus is related to a condition called cochlear synaptopathy, which is characterized by selective loss of synapses between inner hair cells and ribbon synapses.[11],[17],[32] Despite the lack of any prominent and clinically manifested hearing loss symptom, the patient may have a high-frequency (8–16 kHz) hearing loss, which may generally be overlooked by using a conventional audiometric test battery. This high-frequency hearing loss should be considered as the initial finding of the cochlear damage associated-hidden hearing loss and should alert the audiologist while measuring tinnitus in patients with normal conventional pure tone audiometry.[11],[37],[38] We assume that our NH group confirmed this theory.
Generalizability
The heterogeneity and subjectivity of tinnitus can often lead to complexity in studies on this subject, which may adversely affect their comparability. Even calculating the sample size can be challenging. Similarly, some series report cases only with a kind of tinnitus timbre. Sex distribution may vary in different studies. We would like to point out that the sample size in our study was similar to the articles we cited,[4],[6],[7],[8],[9],[10],[13],[14],[15],[21] and additionally, unlike some of these studies, we included patients with both HL and NH. Similarly, the wide spectrum of the tinnitus timbre characteristics of our series and analyzing the data in all aspects can be considered the strength of this study, as well. Therefore, we believe in the reliability and comparability of our results. As known, some authors advocate that tinnitus is dominated by not only a single but also multifactorial pathogenesis, considering its complicated neural process and subjective nature that causes individual differences.[6],[39] In fact, tinnitus is generated by increased auditory spontaneous neural activity and synchrony due to peripheral injury. This is the precondition that underlying both homeostatic plasticity and cortical reorganization theories. However, both of these theories have some limitations. First, it is not clear how spontaneous temporal nerve firing can be responsible for tinnitus. Because, if there was a direct relationship between the tinnitus pitch and temporal firing rate, the tinnitus pitch would be 100 Hz or lower, mimicking the firing rate of the temporal nerve.[6] Second, these theories can only be valid in chronic tinnitus. Because both necessitate time to exist. In addition, the audiogram configuration or the presence of hearing loss might not reflect the actual FMHL, edge frequency, and tinnitus pitch,[6],[40] especially when there is a flat, low frequency, or a profound hearing loss. Our study did not reveal any relation between the audiogram shape and tinnitus pitch, as well. According to our point of view, a more complex and multifactorial mechanism involving the brain should play a role in tinnitus.[34],[41],[42] An elaborative evaluation should be performed on the patients with tinnitus considering all proposed theoretical mechanisms. We assume that the pathophysiology of tinnitus will be better understood in line with the data obtained from patients with the coexistence of different mechanisms, and thus new approaches can be developed for treatment.
Limitations
The primary limitation of this study was the lack of qualitative analysis and questionnaires to assess the individual characteristics of tinnitus perception. In addition, due to the retrospective design of this study, randomization and assessor-blinding were inapplicable. Furthermore, the lack of OAEs and somatosensorial features of tinnitus can be considered minor limitations.
Conclusions | |  |
This research provided evidence for homeostatic plasticity and hidden hearing loss in tinnitus. While the involvement of the tinnitus pitch within the hearing loss zone and close to the FMHL confirmed the presence of homeostatic plasticity, there was a weak correlation between the tinnitus pitch and FMHL, too. Hearing loss seen in high-frequency measurements in individuals with NH in conventional measurements supports the theory of hidden hearing loss/cochlear synaptopathy.
Financial support and sponsorship
Nil.
Conflicts of interest
There are no conflicts of interest.
References | |  |
1. | Eggermont JJ, Roberts LE. The neuroscience of tinnitus. Trends Neurosci 2004;27:676-82. |
2. | Neff P, Langguth B, Schecklmann M, Hannemann R, Schlee W. Comparing three established methods for tinnitus pitch matching with respect to reliability, matching duration, and subjective satisfaction. Trends Hear 2019; 23:1-9. 2331216519887247. |
3. | Ochi K, Ohashi T, Kenmochi M. Hearing impairment and tinnitus pitch in patients with unilateral tinnitus: Comparison of sudden hearing loss and chronic tinnitus. Laryngoscope 2003;113:427-31. |
4. | Sereda M, Hall DA, Bosnyak DJ, Edmondson-Jones M, Roberts LE, Adjamian P, et al. Re-examining the relationship between audiometric profile and tinnitus pitch. Int J Audiol 2011;50:303-12. |
5. | Yakunina N, Nam EC. Does the tinnitus pitch correlate with the frequency of hearing loss? Acta Otolaryngol 2021;141:163-70. |
6. | Pan T, Tyler RS, Ji H, Coelho C, Gehringer AK, Gogel SA. The relationship between tinnitus pitch and the audiogram. Int J Audiol 2009;48:277-94. |
7. | Moore BC, Vinay, Sandhya. The relationship between tinnitus pitch and the edge frequency of the audiogram in individuals with hearing impairment and tonal tinnitus. Hear Res 2010; 261:51-6. |
8. | Shekhawat GS, Searchfield GD, Stinear CM. The relationship between tinnitus pitch and hearing sensitivity. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 2014;271:41-8. |
9. | Keppler H, Degeest S, Dhooge I. The relationship between tinnitus pitch and parameters of audiometry and distortion product otoacoustic emissions. J Laryngol Otol 2017;131:1017-25. |
10. | Schecklmann M, Vielsmeier V, Steffens T, Landgrebe M, Langguth B, Kleinjung T. Relationship between Audiometric slope and tinnitus pitch in tinnitus patients: Insights into the mechanisms of tinnitus generation. PLoS One 2012;7:e34878. |
11. | Xiong B, Liu Z, Liu Q, Peng Y, Wu H, Lin Y, et al. Missed hearing loss in tinnitus patients with normal audiograms. Hear Res 2019;384:107826. |
12. | König O, Schaette R, Kempter R, Gross M. Course of hearing loss and occurrence of tinnitus. Hear Res 2006;221:59-64. |
13. | Raj-Koziak D, Gos E, S´wierniak W, Karpiesz L, Niedziałek I, Włodarczyk E, et al. Relationship between tinnitus loudness measure by visual analogue scale and psychoacoustic matching of tinnitus loudness. Otol Neurotol 2019;40:16-21. |
14. | Sereda M, Edmondson-Jones M, Hall DA. Relationship between tinnitus pitch and edge of hearing loss in individuals with a narrow tinnitus bandwidth. Int J Audiol 2015;54:249-56. |
15. | Savastano M. Tinnitus with or without hearing loss: Are its characteristics different? Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 2008;265:1295-300. |
16. | Martines F, Bentivegna D, Martines E, Sciacca V, Martinciglio G. Assessing audiological, pathophysiological and psychological variables in tinnitus patients with or without hearing loss. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol 2010;267:1685-93. |
17. | Cuesta M, Cobo P. Relating tinnitus features and audiometric characteristics in a cohort of 34 tinnitus subjects. Loquens 2018;5:1-13. |
18. | Ristovska L, Jachova Z, Stojcheska V. Psychoacoustic characteristics of tinnitus in relation to audiometric profile. Arch Acoust 2019;44:419-28. |
19. | Nascimento ID, Almeida AA, Diniz J Jr., Martins ML, Freitas TM, Rosa MR. Tinnitus evaluation: Relationship between pitch matching and loudness, visual analog scale and tinnitus handicap inventory. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol 2019;85:611-6. |
20. | Zagólski O. Management of tinnitus in patients with presbycusis. Int Tinnitus J 2006;12:175-8. |
21. | Martines F, Bentivegna D, Martines E, Sciacca V, Martinciglio G. Characteristics of tinnitus with or without hearing loss: Clinical observations in Sicilian tinnitus patients. Auris Nasus Larynx 2010;37:685-93. |
22. | Flores LS, Teixeira AR, Rosito LP, Seimetz BM, Dall'Igna C. Pitch and loudness from tinnitus in individuals with noise-induced hearing loss. Int Arch Otorhinolaryngol 2016;20:248-53. |
23. | Sultana H, Mumtaz N, Dawood T. Type and degree of hearing loss in patients with tinnitus. Int J Rehabil Sci 2018;07:24-7. |
24. | Lewis RM, Jahn KN, Parthasarathy A, Goedicke WB, Polley DB. Audiometric predictors of bothersome tinnitus in a large clinical cohort of adults with sensorineural hearing loss. Otol Neurotol 2020;41:e414-21. |
25. | Kim TS, Yakunina N, Ryu YJ, Chung IJ, Nam EC. Self-administered tinnitus pitch matching versus a conventional audiometric procedure. Audiol Neurootol 2017;22:1-8. |
26. | Seimetz BM, Teixeira AR, Rosito LP, Flores LS, Pappen CH, Dall'igna C. Pitch and loudness tinnitus in individuals with presbycusis. Int Arch Otorhinolaryngol 2016;20:321-6. |
27. | Mazurek B, Olze H, Haupt H, Szczepek AJ. The more the worse: The grade of noise-induced hearing loss associates with the severity of tinnitus. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2010;7:3071-9. |
28. | Pinto PC, Sanchez TG, Tomita S. The impact of gender, age and hearing loss on tinnitus severity. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol 2010;76:18-24. |
29. | Searchfield GD, Jerram C, Wise K, Raymond S. The impact of hearing loss on tinnitus severity. Aust N Z J Audiol 2007;29:67-76. |
30. | Mahafza N, Zhao F, El Refaie A, Chen F. A comparison of the severity of tinnitus in patients with and without hearing loss using the tinnitus functional index (TFI). Int J Audiol 2021;60:220-6. |
31. | Schaette R, Kempter R. Computational models of neurophysiological correlates of tinnitus. Front Syst Neurosci 2012;6:34. |
32. | Schaette R, Kempter R. Predicting tinnitus pitch from patients' audiograms with a computational model for the development of neuronal hyperactivity. J Neurophysiol 2009;101:3042-52. |
33. | Turrigiano G. Homeostatic synaptic plasticity: Local and global mechanisms for stabilizing neuronal function. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2012;4:a005736. |
34. | Henry JA, Roberts LE, Caspary DM, Theodoroff SM, Salvi RJ. Underlying mechanisms of tinnitus: Review and clinical implications. J Am Acad Audiol 2014;25:5-22. |
35. | Simmons R, Dambra C, Lobarinas E, Stocking C, Salvi R. Head, neck, and eye movements that modulate tinnitus. Semin Hear 2008;29:361-70. |
36. | Çakır A, Ecevit MC, Bal R, Gürkan S, Alpay HC, Şerbetçioğlu MB. Assessment of synaptic plasticity via long-term potentiation in young mice on the day after acoustic trauma: Implications for tinnitus. J Int Adv Otol 2015;11:196-201. |
37. | Schaette R, McAlpine D. Tinnitus with a normal audiogram: Physiological evidence for hidden hearing loss and computational model. J Neurosci 2011;31:13452-7. |
38. | Kim DK, Park SN, Kim HM, Son HR, Kim NG, Park KH, et al. Prevalence and significance of high-frequency hearing loss in subjectively normal-hearing patients with tinnitus. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 2011;120:523-8. |
39. | Baguley DM. Mechanisms of tinnitus. Br Med Bull 2002;63:195-212. |
40. | Moore BC. Psychoacoustics of normal and impaired hearing. Br Med Bull 2002;63:121-34. |
41. | Henry JA, Meikle MB. Psychoacoustic measures of tinnitus. J Am Acad Audiol 2000;11:138-55. |
42. | Henry JA, Dennis KC, Schechter MA. General review of tinnitus: Prevalence, mechanisms, effects, and management. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2005;48:1204-35. |
[Figure 1], [Figure 2], [Figure 3], [Figure 4]
[Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3], [Table 4]
|